Tuesday, November 3, 2009

Self-Hypnosis for Relief of Phantom Limb Pain

Self-hypnosis can be used to relieve phantom limb pain prior or during Mirror Therapy treatment.

Michael F. Garner, a British-certified hypnotherapist, developed effective self-hypnosis scripts for amputees suffering Phantom Limb Pain, focused specifically on arm/hand or leg/foot amputations.


Soothing narratives by Joseph Weisnewski combined with healing musical excerpts by Iqbal Ishani from SleepWell Journey encourage deep relaxation which promotes pain reduction. End The Pain Project offers the self-hypnosis  as mp3 downloads or CDs . 


100% of the proceeds from the sale of a CD or mp3s help support the behind-the-scenes running of ETPP, such as office costs, staff, project costs. Your purchase supports needed non-invasive interventions for amputees suffering Phantom Limb Pain in developing countries.

Conway-Bike eBay Auction for End The Pain Project



It's with deep gratitude that The End The Pain Project's staff has accepted the proceeds from a recent eBay auction for a ConwayQ-RT 1100 Titan Bicycle by Hartje Bicycles of Germany.  This fantastic titanium bike has its seat autographed by the Canadian actor, Stephen Sumner of Battleship Galectica fame.

Mr. Sumner, an ETPP Board member and an above-knee amputee, successfully rode this lightweight unisex bike at the Berlin Velothon, June, 2009. The new German feature film, Phantomschmerz/ Phantom Pain, which premiered at the 2009 Toronto Film Festival, is based upon his experiences.  

Hartje Bicycle's generous donation will fund a pilot project in mid-December.  End The Pain Project hopes to partner with several organizations in Vietnam to deliver Mirror Therapy to land mine amputees utilizing nonbreakable mirror kits for both arm/hand and leg/foot, along with self-hypnosis CDs to be translated into Vietnamese. 

Sunday, November 1, 2009

Mirror Box Therapy from "Phantoms in the Brain" by Blakeslee & Ramachandran






Excerpted from The NY Review of Books Volume 55, Number 11 · June 26, 2008: How the Mind Works: Revelations by Israel Rosenfield & Edward Ziff

The problem of representation, meaning, and memory is also illustrated by the case of a patient who has lost his arm in an accident. As is often the case, the brain creates a "phantom" limb in an apparent attempt to preserve a unified sense of self. For the patient, the phantom limb is painful. The brain knows there is no limb; pain is the consequence of the incoherence between what the brain "sees" (no arm) and the brain's "feeling" the presence of a phantom that it has created in its attempt to maintain a unified sense of self in continuity with the past. Such pain is not created by an external stimulus and cannot be eliminated by painkillers.

One famous case is that of a young man who had lost his hand in a motorcycle accident. In a therapeutic procedure devised by V.S. Ramachandran, and described in his book with Sandra Blakeslee, Phantoms in the Brain, the patient put his intact hand in one side of a box and "inserted" his phantom hand in the other side. As the illustration on this page shows, one section of the box had a vertical mirror, which showed a reflection of his intact hand. The patient observed in the mirror the image of his real hand, and was then asked to make similar movements with both "hands," which suggested to the brain real movement from the lost hand. Suddenly the pain disappeared. Though the young man was perfectly aware of the trick being played on him—the stump of his amputated arm was lying in one section of the box—the visual image overcame his sense of being tricked. Seeing is believing! Pain—the consequence of the incoherence between the brain's creation of a phantom limb and the visual realization that the limb does not exist—disappeared; what was seen (a hand in the mirror) matched what was felt (a phantom).

According to the Italian neurologist Angela Sirigu, who used videos instead of mirrors to perform a similar experiment,

It is the dissonance between the image of oneself and the damaged body, that is at the origin of the phantom pain. Seeing the damaged hand once again functioning, reduces the dissonance even though the patient is aware of being tricked.

At one moment the patient experiences a painful phantom limb; at another he sees a mirror image of his intact hand and the pain disappears. This is only one of many neurological examples of what we might call the Dr. Jekyll and Mr. Hyde Syndrome: the patient in the experiment sees and remembers one world at certain times and a completely different world at other times.[4] The phantom limb is the brain's way of preserving a body image—a sense of self that is essential to all coherent brain activity. And as in the case of colors, the phantom limb suggests that what we see, hear, and feel are inventions of the brain—an integration of the past (the loss of the limb) and the present (a phantom that is essential for the brain's continuing to function "normally").

In general, every recollection refers not only to the remembered event or person or object but to the person who is remembering. The very essence of memory is subjective, not mechanical, reproduction; and essential to that subjective psychology is that every remembered image of a person, place, idea, or object inevitably contains, whether explicitly or implicitly, a basic reference to the person who is remembering.

Our conscious life is a constant flow, or integration, of an immediate past and the present—what Henri Bergson called le souvenir du prĂ©sent (1908) and Edelman more recently called the remembered present (1989). Consciousness, in this view, is neither recalled representations nor the immediate present, but something different in kind (as colors are different in kind from the lightness and darkness of different reflected wavelengths).

The importance of body image and motor activity for perception, physical movement, and thought is suggested by the recent discovery of "mirror neurons" by Giacomo Rizzolatti and his colleagues. They observed that the neurons that fired when a monkey grasped an object also fired when the monkey watched a scientist grasp the same object. The monkey apparently understood the action of the experimenter because the activity within its brain was similar when the monkey was observing the experimenter and when the monkey was grasping the object. What was surprising was that the same neurons that produced "motor actions," i.e., actions involving muscular movement, were active when the monkey was perceiving those actions performed by others.


Phantoms in the Brain: Probing the Mysteries of the Human Mind
by V.S. Ramachandran and Sandra Blakeslee, with a foreword by Oliver Sacks
Quill, 328 pp., $16.00 (paper)

Mirrors in the Brain: How Our Minds Share Actions and and Emotions
by Giacomo Rizzolatti and Corrado Sinigaglia, translated from the Italian by Frances Anderson
Oxford University Press,242 pp., $49.95

A Universe of Consciousness: How Matter Becomes Imagination
by Gerald M. Edelman and Giulio Tononi
Basic Books, 274 pp., $18.00 (paper)